TWO of a KIND!


DOUBLE TROUBLE.


Twinsburg, Ohio


* * * * * * * *



AUGUST 14th, 2004


RESEARCHERS HAVE DESCENDED ON A SMALL TOWN IN OHIO FOR TWO FRENZIED DAYS OF WORK. TWIN RESEARCH HAS NEVER BEEN SO POPULAR.



IN THE FIRST WEEKEND OF EVERY AUGUST, the town of Twinsburg, Ohio, holds a parade . Decorated floats, cars and lorries roll slowly past neat, white houses and clipped lawns, while thousands of onlookers clap and wave flags in the sunshine.


The scene is a perfect little slice of America. There is, though, something rather strange about the participants: they all seem to come in pairs. Identical twins of all colours, shapes, ages and sizes are assembling for the world’s largest annual gathering of their kind.


The Twinsburg meeting is of interest to more people than just the twins themselves. Every year, the festival attracts dozens of scientists who come to prod, swab, sample and question the participants. For identical twins are natural clones: the odd mutation aside, they share 100% of their genes.


That means studying them can cast light on the relative importance of genetics and environment in shaping particular human characteristics.


In the past, such research has been controversial. Josef Mengele, a Nazi doctor working at the Auschwitz extermination camp during the second world war, was fascinated by twins. He sought them out among arrivals at the camp and preserved them from the gas-chambers for a series of brutal experiments. After the war, Cyril Burt, a British psychologist who worked on the heredity of intelligence, tainted twin research with results that appear, in retrospect, to have been rather too good. Some of his data on identical twins who had been reared apart were probably faked.


 In any case, the prevailing ideology in the social sciences after the war was Marxist, and disliked suggestions that differences in human potential might have underlying genetic causes. Twin studies were thus viewed with suspicion.


Womb mates


The ideological pendulum has swung back, however, as the human genome project and its aftermath have turned genes from abstract concepts to real pieces of DNA. The role of genes in sensitive areas such as intelligence is acknowledged by all but a few die-bards . The interesting questions now concern how nature and nurture interact to produce particular bits of biology, rather than which of the two is more important . Twin studies, which are a good way to ask these questions, are back in fashion, and many twins are enthusiastic participants in this research.


Laura and Linda Seber, for example, are identical twins from Sheffield Village, Ohio. They have been coming to Twinsburg for decades. Over the years, they have taken part in around 50 experiments. They have had their every reaction measured, been deprived of sleep for a night and had electrodes attached to their brains. Like many other twins, they do it because they find the tests interesting and want to help science.


Research at the Twinsburg festival began in a small way, with a single stand in 1979. Gradually, news spread, and more scientists began turning up. This year, half a dozen groups of researchers were lodged in a specially pitched research tent.


In one corner of this tent, Paul Breslin, who works at the Monell Institute in Philadelphia, watched over several tables where twins sat sipping clear liquids from cups and making notes. It was the team’s third year at Twinsburg. Dr Breslin and his colleagues want to find out how genes influence human perception, particularly the senses of smell and taste and those (warmth, cold, pain, tingle, itch and so on) that result from stimulation of the skin.



Perception is an example of something that is probably influenced by both genes and experience. Even before birth, people are exposed to flavours such as chocolate, garlic, mint and vanilla that pass intact into the bloodstream, and thus to the fetus.


Though it is not yet clear whether such prenatal exposure shapes taste-perception, there is evidence that it shapes preferences for foods encountered later in life.


However, there are clearly genetic influences at work, as well—for example in the ability to taste quinine. Some people experience this as intensely bitter, even when it is present at very low levels. Others, whose genetic endowment is different, are less bothered by it. Twin studies make this extremely clear. Within a pair of identical twins, either both, or neither, will find quinine hard to swallow. Non-identical twins will agree less frequently.

 

On the other side of the tent Dennis Drayna, ftom the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, in Maryland, was studying hearing. He wants to know what happens to sounds after they reach the ear. It is not clear, he says, whether sound is processed into sensation mostly in the ear or in the brain. Dr Drayna has already been involved in a twin study which revealed that the perception of musical pitch is highly heritable. At Twinsburg, he is playing different words, or parts of words, into the left and right ears of his twinned volunteers.

 

The composite of the two sounds that an individual reports hearing depends on how he processes this diverse information and that, Dr Drayna believes, may well be influenced by genetics.

 

Elsewhere in the marquee, Peter Miraldi, of Kent State University in Ohio, was trying to find out whether genes affect an individual’s motivation to communicate with others. A number of twin studies have shown that personality and sociability are heritable, so he thinks this is fertile ground. And next to Mr Miraldi was a team of dermatologists from Case Western Reserve University in Cleveland. They are looking at the development of skin diseases and male-pattern baldness. The goal of the latter piece of research is to find the genes responsible for making men’s hair

fallout.

 

The busiest part of the tent, however, was the queue for forensic-science research into fingerprints. The origins of this study are shrouded in mystery. For many months, the festival’s organisers have been convinced that the Secret Service—the American government agency responsible for, among other things, the safety of the president—is behind it.

 

When The Economist contacted the Secret Service for more information, we were referred to Steve Nash, who is chairman of the International Association for Identification (IAI), and is also a detective in the scientific investigations section of the Mann County Sheriff’s Office in California. The IAI based in Minnesota, is an organization of forensic scientists from around the world. Among other things, it publishes the Journal of Forensic Identification.

 

Mr. Nash insists that the work on twins is being sponsored by the IAI, and so has nothing to do with the Secret Service. He says the organization collects sets of similar finger and palm prints so that improvements can be made in the ability to distinguish ordinary sets of prints. Although identical twins tend to share whorls, loops and arches in their fingerprints because of their common heredity, the precise patterns of their prints are not the same.

 

Just who will benefit from this research is unclear. Although the IAI is an internaTional organization, not everyone in it will have access to the twin data. Deciding who does will have “a lot of parameters”, according to Mr Nash. He says that the work is being assisted by the American government at the county, state and federal level, and that government agencies will have access to the data for their research.

 

He takes pains to stress that this will be for research purposes, and says none of the data will be included in any criminal data-bases. But this cloak-and-dagger approach suggests that, while twin studies have come a long way, they have not shaken off their controversial past quite yet. If they are truly to do so, a little more openness from the Feds would be nice.

 


 

 

 

CLAIMING ONE’S INHERITANCE.

 

The relationship between genes and experience is becoming much better understood!

 

T HE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF TWINS GOES BACK TO THE LATE 19TH CENTURY, WHEN FRANCIS GALTON, AN EARLY GENETICIST, REALISED THAT THEY CAME IN TWO VARIETIES: IDENTICAL TWINS BORN FROM ONE EGG AND NON-IDENTICAL TWINS THAT HAD COME FROM TWO.

 

THAT INSIGHT TURNED OUT TO BE THE KEY, ALTHOUGH IT WAS STILL NOT UNTIL 1924 THAT IT WAS USED TO FORMULATE WHAT IS NOW KNOWN AS THE TWIN RULE OF PATHOLOGY, AND TWIN STUDIES FINALLY REALLY GOT GOING.

 

 

The twin rule of pathology states that any heritable disease will be more concordant (that is, more likely to be jointly present or absent) in identical twins than in non-identical twins---—and, in turn, will be more concordant in non-identical twins than in non-siblings.

 

Early work, for example, showed that the statistical correlation of skin-mole counts between identical twins was 0.4, while non-identical twins had a correlation of only 0.2. (A score of 1.0 implies perfect correlation, while a score of zero implies no correlation.) This result suggests that moles are heritable, but it also implies that there is an environmental component to the development of moles, otherwise the correlation in identical twins would be close to 1.0.

 

Twin research has shown that whether or not someone takes up smoking is also determined mainly by environmental factors, but once he does so, how much he smokes is largely down to his genes.

 

And while a person’s religion is clearly a cultural attribute, there is a strong genetic component to religious fundamentalism. Twin studies are also unravelling the heritability of various aspects of human personality. Traits from neuroticism and anxiety to thrill- and-novelty-seeking all have large genetic components. Parenting matters, but it does not determine personality in the way that some had thought.

 

More importantly, perhaps, twin studies are helping the understanding of diseases such as cancer, asthma, osteoporosis, arthrifis and immune disorders. And twins can be used, within ethical limits, for medical experiments. A recent study that administered vitamin C to one twin and a placebo to the other found that it had no effect on the common cold.

 

The lesson from all today’s twin studies is that most human traits are at least just partially influenced by genes . However, for the most part, the age-old dichotomy between nature and nurture is not very useful. Many genetic programs are open to input from the environment, and genes are frequently switched on or off by the environmental signals. It is also possible that genes themselves influence their environment Some humans have an innate preference for participation in sports. Others are drawn to novelty. Might people also be drawn to certain kinds of friends and types of experience? In this way, a person’s genes might shape the very environment they act in as much as the environment shapes the actions of the genes.

 

SOURCE:

The Economist Magazine

August 14th-20th 2004 (Pgs. 70-71)

www.economist.com



bar_blbk.jpg - 5566 Bytes

Return to the Words of Wisdom, science menu..

Return to the main menu..

D.U.O Project
Church of the Science of God
La Jolla, California 92038-3131
(858) 220-1604

Church of the Science of GOD, 1993
Web Designed by WebDiva